The Stroop effect is a demonstration of the phenomenon that the brain's reaction time slows down when it has to deal with conflicting information. This slowed reaction time happens because of interference, or a processing delay caused by competing or incompatible functions in the brain. The effect became widely known after John Ridley Stroop, an American psychologist, published a paper on it in 1935, but it had been studied by several other researchers before Stroop.
This phenomenon is typically studied with a Stroop test. In this test, a researcher times how long it takes a test taker to say the name of a color printed in gray or black ink. For instance, the person would see the word "blue" printed and then say "blue." The researcher then shows the test taker color names printed in another color — like the word "green" printed in orange ink — and times how long it takes the test taker to say the color that the word is printed in. Most people are much slower and are more likely to make mistakes during the second task than the first one since the second one presents the brain with conflicting information. Stroop's original test was a little different, but many modern Stroop tests are structured in this way.
An example of the second section of a Stroop test might be:
red | orange | white | green |
yellow | brown | orange | white |
blue | purple | black | red |
brown | green | orange | yellow |
white | red | purple | green |
During a Stroop test, two parts of the brain's frontal lobe — the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex — come into play. Both are connected to catching errors and resolving conflicts, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is also involved with memory and organization, among other things.
Possible Explanations
There are two main theories used to explain the Stroop effect, but no one final explanation. The theories are:
- Speed of processing theory: the brain reads words faster than it recognizes colors, so there's a lag while the brain recognizes the color.
- Selective attention theory: the brain needs to use more attention to recognize a color than to read a word, so it takes a little longer.
Other theories:
- Automation of reading theory/Automaticity Hypothesis: the brain automatically understands the meaning of words through a long-time habit of reading, but recognizing colors is not an automatic process. When the brain has to name the color, rather than the meaning of the word in the Stroop test it has to override its initial impulse of automatically reading the word rather so that it can recognize its color.
- Bottleneck theory: the brain analyzes most streams of information unconsciously through automatic processes that are hard to control. Processes like color recognition require the brain's attention, but the unconscious processes can disturb that attention, which accounts for the delay.
- Parallel distributed processing theory: as the brain analyzes information, it builds up specific pathways for doing each task. Some pathways, like reading, are stronger than others, like naming colors. So when two pathways are activated simultaneously in the Stroop test, interference occurs between the stronger “reading” path and the weaker “color naming” path.
Uses of the Stroop Effect
The Stroop effect is used in variations of Stroop tests to measure many different things, including how well a person's selective attention works and his or her brain's processing speed. It is also used as part of a group of tests for a person's executive processing, which is basically how good one part of the brain is at managing the other parts. A researcher might also give a Stroop test to a person while also giving him or her a brain scan to see what parts of the brain are involved in things like color recognition or managing interference as a way to study the brain. Tests like this can also shed light on how people would handle interference in other situations, for instance, while texting and driving.
Stroop tests are also used as tools for screening people and diagnosing certain mental problems, including dementia, schizophrenia, brain damage after a stroke, and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). This can help doctors test certain aspects of how a patient's brain is functioning, particularly aspects related to attention and focusing. For instance, schizophrenics tend to show more interference when taking Stroop tests than those without schizophrenia because that condition makes it hard for the brain to focus and filter some types of information.
Variations on the Stroop Effect
This effect isn't limited to just colors; it has also been shown to have an effect in tests where the words are turned upside down, or at odd angles. Other Stroop tests are based around emotions. For instance, a researcher might show a person cards with words like "depression," "war," and "pain" mixed in with more neutral words like "watch," "doorknob," and "box". Just like in a normal Stroop test, the words are colored and the test taker is supposed to name the color. The researcher then times the test taker to see if the test taker said the sad words faster or slower than the neutral words.
There is also a phenomenon called the reverse Stroop effect, where test takers are shown a page with a black square with a mismatching colored word in the middle — for instance, the word "blue" written in the color red — with four smaller colored squares in the corners. One square would be colored red, one square would be blue, and the two others would be other colors. Experiments show that if the test takers are asked to point to the color box of the written color, blue, they have a delay just as though they were taking a classic Stroop test in which they were supposed to say the shown color of the word, in this case, red.
Additional Resources:
http://psychclassics.yorku.ca — Stroop's original paper.
http://snre.umich.edu — More detailed information about the physiology behind this effect with more quizzes.
http://www.rit.edu — Information about alterative theories on the Stroop effect.
http://www.swarthmore.edu — A paper on the reverse Stroop effect.